Ruth Byrne, Trinity College Dublin and Kinga Morsanyi, Queen's University Belfast
Sixteen-year-old climate activist Greta Thunberg has made headlines not just for her passionate climate activism, but because she is outspoken about having autism. However, her treatment by some members of the media – even being called “mentally ill” – shows that many harmful myths about autism still persist, despite increased awareness.
About one in every 60 people has autism. Although each person is unique, autistic people share some common characteristics. These include difficulty with social interactions, repetitive behaviour and restricted interests, which are present from early childhood and limit everyday functioning. Autism is a spectrum condition, which means the type and severity of symptoms varies depending on the person.
In our new book, we invited leading experts in cognitive science and autism to present their research. This research gives new insights into the autistic mind and what it’s like to have autism. It dispels common assumptions about this form of atypical development.
Can autistic people make good decisions?
Everyday activities, such as going shopping or visiting the hairdresser, can often be challenging for autistic people. For example, they are more likely than the average person to report buying things they don’t use. They often find it difficult to make small decisions such as what clothes to wear or what to eat. But when it comes to making big decisions, such as about who to marry or where to work, they do so in the same way as a typical person.
In our book, we present research showing that autistic people tend to base their decisions on careful reflection. This might be because they’re less likely to rely on their feelings and intuitions compared with the average person. As a result they take longer to make decisions and they don’t tend to jump to conclusions as much as the typical person.
These examples show that whether someone with autism makes “better” or “worse” decisions compared to others depends on the kind of decision they’re making. Indeed, in many cases, their choices aren’t better or worse than the typical person’s – just different. For example, they’re more likely to buy something from an ad that features a person enjoying the product on their own rather than with others.
Can autistic people be imaginative?
It’s often assumed that autistic people lack imagination because of their focus on exact details and facts. For the average person, being able to imagine alternatives to reality is easy, whether ruminating on past events or daydreaming about how the future might unfold. Even very young children, between one-and-a-half to two years old, begin to engage in pretend play.
Contrary to common belief, children with autism do develop these rational imagination skills – although it might take them two or three years longer than other children.
Likewise, analogical thinking, in which a person compares two objects or events, is considered essential to creativity and for understanding new concepts. Remarkably, autistic people often show outstanding skills at solving pictorial analogies – such as finding the hidden pattern in the Raven’s Matrices test. Despite some differences in the development of creative thought, autistic people have as rich an imaginative mental life as anyone.
Do autistic people interpret things literally?
There’s a persistent idea that autistic people interpret everything literally. In fact, an inability to understand metaphors and other forms of non-literal language is part of the diagnostic criteria for autism.
But autistic people do understand the real meaning of metaphors just as non-autistic people do, when they are compared with people with similar language abilities. They also understand that indirect requests, such as: “Can you close the window?”, require an action rather than a “yes” or “no” answer.
Autistic people can rely on background knowledge to know which logical inferences to make – though they sometimes do so differently from the average person. For example, if they’re told: “If Lisa has an essay to write she will study late in the library” and: “If the library stays open she will study late in the library”, they often infer that: “She will study late in the library”. Given the same information, the average person doesn’t usually infer that Lisa will study late in the library, because they recognise that they don’t know whether the library stayed open or not.
Autistic people sometimes differ from others in how they combine different sorts of knowledge. Nevertheless, in most cases they get the big picture and can usually uncover the hidden meaning of what someone has said to them.
These new discoveries contradict some current stereotypes of autism, revealing that the thought processes of autistic people are not entirely different from the average person’s. They also show how these differences can be advantageous in some situations. For example, careful decision making is useful when deciding who to vote for, or what investment to make. But it can be a drawback in a situation that calls for a rapid response, such as when a person needs to think on their feet in a job interview.
Autism diagnoses are steadily increasing around the world, though many people still remain undiagnosed. The discoveries in our book help build a deeper understanding of the autistic mind – although the reasons for some autistic characteristics still remain unknown. The contribution of autistic people discussing their experiences with autism, helps dispel persistent misconceptions about this form of atypical development.
Amendment. The term “people with autism” has been replaced throughout with “autistic people”. And the term “developmental disorder” has been replaced with “atypical development”.
Ruth Byrne, Professor of Cognitive Science in the School of Psychology and Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin and Kinga Morsanyi, Lecturer in the School of Psychology, Queen's University Belfast
This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.